Scientific misconduct

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by 130.226.154.66 (talk) at 09:17, 8 July 2012 (→‎Denmark). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

Scientific misconduct is the violation of the standard codes of scholarly conduct and ethical behavior in professional scientific research. A Lancet review on Handling of Scientific Misconduct in Scandinavian countries provides the following sample definitions:[1] (reproduced in The COPE report 1999[2])

  • Danish definition: "Intention or gross negligence leading to fabrication of the scientific message or a false credit or emphasis given to a scientist"
  • Swedish definition: "Intention[al] distortion of the research process by fabrication of data, text, hypothesis, or methods from another researcher's manuscript form or publication; or distortion of the research process in other ways."

The consequences of scientific misconduct can be damaging for both perpetrators[3][4] and any individual who exposes it[5]. In addition there are public health implications attached to the promotion of medical or other interventions based on dubious research findings.

Motivation to commit scientific misconduct

According to David Goodstein of Caltech, there are motivators for scientists to commit misconduct, which are briefly summarised here.[6]

Career pressure
Science is still a very strongly career-driven discipline. Scientists depend on a good reputation to receive ongoing support and funding, and a good reputation relies largely on the publication of high-profile scientific papers. Hence, there is a strong imperative to "publish or perish". Clearly, this may motivate desperate (or fame-hungry) scientists to fabricate results.
To this category may also be added a paranoia that there are other scientists out there who are close to success in the same experiment, which puts extra pressure on being the first one. It is suggested as a cause of the fraud of Hwang Woo-Suk.[citation needed][7] A main source of detection comes when other research teams in fact fail or get different results.
Ease of fabrication
In many scientific fields, results are often difficult to reproduce accurately, being obscured by noise, artifacts, and other extraneous data. That means that even if a scientist does falsify data, he can expect to get away with it – or at least claim innocence if his results conflict with others in the same field. There are no "scientific police" who are trained to fight scientific crimes; all investigations are made by experts in science but amateurs in dealing with criminals. It is relatively easy to cheat although difficult to know exactly how many scientists fabricate data.[8]

Forms of scientific misconduct

The U.S. National Science Foundation defines three types of research misconduct: fabrication, falsification, and plagiarism[9][10]

  • Fabrication is making up results and recording or reporting them. This is sometimes referred to as "drylabbing"[11]. A more minor form of fabrication is where references are included to give arguments the appearance of widespread acceptance, but are actually fake, and/or do not support the argument[12].
  • Falsification is manipulating research materials, equipment, or processes or changing or omitting data or results such that the research is not accurately represented in the research record.
  • Plagiarism is the appropriation of another person’s ideas, processes, results, or words without giving appropriate credit. One form is the appropriation of the ideas and results of others, and publishing as to make it appear the author had performed all the work under which the data was obtained. A subset is citation plagiarism – willful or negligent failure to appropriately credit other or prior discoverers, so as to give an improper impression of priority. This is also known as, "citation amnesia", the "disregard syndrome" and "bibliographic negligence"[13] Arguably, this is the most common type of scientific misconduct. Sometimes it is difficult to guess whether authors intentionally ignored a highly relevant cite or lacked knowledge of the prior work. Discovery credit can also be inadvertently reassigned from the original discoverer to a better-known researcher. This is a special case of the Matthew effect.[14]
    • Plagiarism-Fabrication - the act of taking an unrelated figure from an unrelated publication and reproducing it exactly in a new publication (claiming that it represents new data). Recent papers from the University of Cordoba have come to light showing how this can go undetected and unchallenged for years[15][16].
    • Self-plagiarism – or multiple publication of the same content with different titles and/or in different journals is sometimes also considered misconduct; scientific journals explicitly ask authors not to do this. It is referred to as "salami" (i.e. many identical slices) in the jargon of medical journal editors (MJE). According to some MJE this includes publishing the same article in a different language[17].
  • The violation of ethical standards regarding human and animal experiments – such as the standard that a human subject of the experiment must give informed consent to the experiment.[18]. Failure to obtain ethical approval for clinical studies characterised the case of Joachim Boldt.
  • Ghostwriting – the phenomenon where someone other than the named author(s) makes a major contribution. Typically, this is done to mask contributions from drug companies. It incorporates plagiarism and has an additional element of financial fraud.
  • Conversely, research misconduct is not limited to NOT listing authorship, but also includes the conferring authorship on those that have not made substantial contributions to the research.[19][20] This is done by senior researchers who muscle their way onto the papers of inexperienced junior researchers[21] as well as others that stack authorship in an effort to guarantee publication. This is much harder to prove due to a lack of consistency in defining "authorship" or "substantial contribution".[22][23][24]

In addition, some academics consider suppression—the failure to publish significant findings due to the results being adverse to the interests of the researcher or his/her sponsor(s)—to be a form of misconduct as well. ** Bare assertions – making entirely unsubstantiated claims - may also be considered a form of research misconduct although there is no evidence that cases of this form have ever led to a finding of misconduct.

In some cases, scientific misconduct may also constitute violations of the law, but not always. Being accused of the activities described in this article is a serious matter for a practicing scientist, with severe consequences should it be determined that a researcher intentionally or carelessly engaged in misconduct. However in most countries, committing research misconduct, even on a large scale, is not a legal offence.

Three percent of the 3,475 research institutions that report to the US Department of Health and Human Services' Office of Research Integrity, indicate some form of scientific misconduct.[25] However the ORI will only investigate allegations of impropriety where research was funded by federal grants. They routinely monitor such research publication for red flags. Other private organizations like the Committee of Medical Journal Editors (COJE) can only police their own members.

The validity of the methods and results of scientific papers are often scrutinized in journal clubs. In this venue, members can decide amongst themselves with the help of peers if a scientific paper's ethical standards are met.

Responsibility of authors and of coauthors

Authors and coauthors of scientific publications have a variety of responsibilities. Contravention of the rules of scientific authorship may lead to a charge of scientific misconduct. All authors, including coauthors, are expected to have made reasonable attempts to check findings submitted to academic journals for publication. Simultaneous submission of scientific findings to more than one journal or duplicate publication of findings is usually regarded as misconduct, under what is known as the Ingelfinger rule, named after the editor of the New England Journal of Medicine 1967-1977, Franz Ingelfinger.[26]

Guest authorship (where there is stated authorship in the absence of involvement, also known as gift authorship) and ghost authorship (where the real author is not listed as an author) are commonly regarded as forms of research misconduct. In some cases coauthors of faked research have been accused of inappropriate behavior or research misconduct for failing to verify reports authored by others or by a commercial sponsor. Examples include the case of Gerald Schatten who co-authored with Hwang Woo-Suk, the case of Professor Geoffrey Chamberlain named as guest author of papers fabricated by Malcolm Pearce,[27] (Chamberlain was exonerated from collusion in Pearce's deception)[28] - and the coauthors with Jan Hendrik Schön at Bell Laboratories. More recent cases include that of Charles Nemeroff, then the editor-in-chief of Neuropsychopharmacology, and a well-documented case involving the drug Actonel.

Authors are expected to keep all study data for later examination even after publication. The failure to keep data may be regarded as misconduct. Some scientific journals require that authors provide information to allow readers to determine whether the authors might have commercial or non-commercial conflicts of interest. Authors are also commonly required to provide information about ethical aspects of research, particularly where research involves human or animal participants or use of biological material. Provision of incorrect information to journals may be regarded as misconduct. Financial pressures on universities have encouraged this type of misconduct. The majority of recent cases of alleged misconduct involving undisclosed conflicts of interest or failure of the authors to have seen scientific data involve collaborative research between scientists and biotechnology companies (Nemeroff, Blumsohn).

Responsibilities of research institutions

In general, defining whether an individual is guilty of misconduct requires a detailed investigation by the individual's employing academic institution. Such investigations require detailed and rigorous processes and can be extremely costly. Furthermore, the more senior the individual under suspicion, the more likely it is that conflicts of interest will compromise the investigation. In many countries (with the notable exception of the United States) acquisition of funds on the basis of fraudulent data is not a legal offence and there is consequently no regulator to oversee investigations into alleged research misconduct. Universities therefore have few incentives to investigate allegations in a robust manner, or act on the findings of such investigations if they vindicate the allegation.

Well publicised cases illustrate the potential role that senior academics in research institutions play in concealing scientific misconduct. A King's College (London) internal investigation showed research findings from one of their researchers to be 'at best unreliable, and in many cases spurious' [29] but the college took no action e.g. retracting relevant published research, or preventing further episodes from occurring. It was only 10 years later, when an entirely separate form of misconduct by the same individual was being investigated by the General Medical Council, that the internal report came to light.

In a more recent case an internal investigation at the National Centre for Cell Science (NCCS), Pune determined that there was evidence of misconduct by Dr. Gopal Kundu, but an external committee was then organised which dismissed the allegation, and the NCCS issued a memorandum exonerating the authors of all charges of misconduct. Undeterred by the NCCS exoneration, the relevant journal (Journal of Biological Chemistry) withdrew the paper based on its own analysis.

Responsibilities of scientific colleagues who are "bystanders"

Some academics believe that scientific colleagues who suspect scientific misconduct should consider taking informal action themselves, or reporting their concerns. (See Gerald Koocher and Patricia Keith Speigel in NATURE Vol 466 22 July 2010: Peers Nip Misconduct in the Bud, and (with Joan Sieber) Responding to Research Wrongdoing: A User Friendly Guide, July 2010.) This question is of great importance since much research suggests that it is very difficult for people to act or come forward when they see unacceptable behavior, unless they have help from their organizations. A "User-friendly Guide," and the existence of a confidential organizational ombudsman may help people who are uncertain about what to do, or afraid of bad consequences for their speaking up. (See Mary Rowe, Linda Wilcox and Howard Gadlin, Dealing with—or Reporting—"Unacceptable" Behavior—with additional thoughts about the "Bystander Effect," in JIOA, vol.2, no.1, pp52–62.)

Responsibility of journals

Journals are responsible for safeguarding the research record and hence have a critical role in dealing with suspected misconduct. This is recognised by the Committee on Publication Ethics (COPE) which has issued clear guidelines on the form (e.g. retraction) that concerns over the research record should take.

  • The COPE guidelines state that journal editors should consider retracting a publication if they have clear evidence that the findings are unreliable, either as a result of misconduct (e.g. data fabrication) or honest error (e.g. miscalculation or experimental error). Retraction is also appropriate in cases of redundant publication, plagiarism and unethical research.
  • Journal editors should consider issuing an expression of concern if they receive inconclusive evidence of research or publication misconduct by the authors, there is evidence that the findings are unreliable but the authors’ institution will not investigate the case, they believe that an investigation into alleged misconduct related to the publication either has not been, or would not be, fair and impartial or conclusive, or an investigation is underway but a judgement will not be available for a considerable time.
  • Journal editors should consider issuing a correction if a small portion of an otherwise reliable publication proves to be misleading (especially because of honest error), or the author / contributor list is incorrect (i.e. a deserving author has been omitted or somebody who does not meet authorship criteria has been included).

Recent evidence has emerged that journals learning of cases where there is strong evidence of possible misconduct, with issues potentially affecting a large portion of the findings, frequently fail to issue an expression of concern or correspond with the host institution so that an investigation can be undertaken. In one case the Journal of Clinical Oncology issued a Correction despite strong evidence that the original paper was invalid[16]. In another case[30], Nature allowed a Corrigendum to be published despite clear evidence of image fraud. Subsequent Retraction of the paper required the actions of an independent whistleblower[31].

The recent cases of Joachim Boldt and Yoshitaka Fujii in anaesthesiology have focussed attention on the role that journals play in perpetuating scientific fraud as well as how they can deal with it. In the Boldt case, the Editors-in-Chief of 18 specialist journals (generally anaesthesia and intensive care) made a joint statement regarding 88 published clinical trials conducted without Ethics Committee approval. In the Fujii case, involving nearly 200 papers, the journal Anesthesia & Analgesia, which published 24 of Fujii’s papers, has accepted that its handling of the issue was inadequate. Following publication of a Letter to the Editor from Kranke and colleagues in April 2000[32], along with a non-specific response from Dr. Fujii, there was no follow-up on the allegation of data manipulation and no request for an institutional review of Dr. Fujii’s research. Anesthesia & Analgesia went on to publish 11 additional manuscripts by Dr. Fujii following the 2000 allegations of research fraud, with Editor Steven Shafer stating in March 2012 that subsequent submissions to the Journal by Dr. Fujii should not have been published without first vetting the allegations of fraud. In April 2012 Shafer led a group of editors to write a joint statement, in the form of an ultimatum made available to the public, to a large number of academic institutions where Fujii had been employed, offering these institutions the chance to attest to the integrity of the bulk of the allegedly fraudulent papers.

Photo manipulation

Compared to other forms of scientific misconduct, image fraud (manipulation of images to distort their meaning) is of particular interest since it can frequently be detected by external parties. In 2006, the Journal of Cell Biology gained publicity for instituting tests to detect photo manipulation in papers that were being considered for publication.[33] This was in response to the increased usage of programs by scientists such as Adobe Photoshop, which facilitate photo manipulation. Since then more publishers, including the Nature Publishing Group, have instituted similar tests and require authors to minimize and specify the extent of photo manipulation when a manuscript is submitted for publication. However there is little evidence to indicate that such tests are applied rigorously. One Nature paper published in 2009[30] has subsequently been reported to contain around 20 separate instances of image fraud.

Although the type of manipulation that is allowed can depend greatly on the type of experiment that is presented and also differ from one journal to another, in general the following manipulations are not allowed:[citation needed]

  • splicing together different images to represent a single experiment
  • changing brightness and contrast of only a part of the image
  • any change that conceals information, even when it is considered to be aspecific, which includes:
    • changing brightness and contrast to leave only the most intense signal
    • using clone tools to hide information
  • showing only a very small part of the photograph so that additional information is not visible

Suppression/non-publication of data

A related issue concerns the deliberate suppression, failure to publish, or selective release of the findings of scientific studies. Such cases may not be strictly definable as scientific misconduct as the deliberate falsification of results is not present. However, in such cases the intent may nevertheless be to deliberately deceive. Studies may be suppressed or remain unpublished because the findings are perceived to undermine the commercial, political or other interests of the sponsoring agent or because they fail to support the ideological goals of the researcher. Examples include the failure to publish studies if they demonstrate the harm of a new drug, or truthfully publishing the benefits of a treatment while omitting harmful side-effects.

This is distinguishable from other concepts such as bad science, junk science or pseudoscience where the criticism centres on the methodology or underlying assumptions. It may be possible in some cases to use statistical methods to show that the datasets offered in relation to a given field are incomplete. However this may simply reflect the existence of real-world restrictions on researchers without justifying more sinister conclusions.

Some cases go beyond the failure to publish complete reports of all findings with researchers knowingly making false claims based on falsified data. This falls clearly under the definition of scientific misconduct, even if the result was achieved by suppressing data. In the case of Raphael B. Stricker, M.D.,[34] for instance, the U.S. Office of Research Integrity has found the removal of samples from a data set in order to reach a desired conclusion to be grounds for disbarment from funding.

Consequences for science

The consequences of scientific fraud vary based on the severity of the fraud, the level of notice it receives, and how long it goes undetected. For cases of fabricated evidence, the consequences can be wide ranging, with others working to confirm (or refute) the false finding, or with research agendas being distorted to address the fraudulent evidence. The Piltdown Man fraud is a case in point: The significance of the bona-fide fossils' being found was muted for decades because they disagreed with Piltdown Man and the pre-conceived notions that those faked fossils supported. In addition, the prominent paleontologist Arthur Smith Woodward spent time at Piltdown each year until he died trying to find more Piltdown Man remains. The misdirection of resources kept others from taking the real fossils more seriously and delayed the reaching of a correct understanding of human evolution. (The Taung Child, which should have been the death knell for the view that the human brain evolved first, was instead treated very critically because of its disagreement with the Piltdown Man evidence.)

In the case of Dr Alfred Steinschneider, two decades and tens of millions of research dollars were lost trying to find the elusive link between infant sleep apnea, that Steinschneider said he had observed and recorded in his laboratory and claimed was a precursor of sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS). The cover was blown in 1994, 22 years after Steinschneider's 1972 Pediatrics paper claiming such an association,[35] when Waneta Hoyt, the mother of the patients in the paper, was arrested, indicted and convicted on 5 counts of second degree manslaughter for the smothering deaths of her five children.[36] While that in itself was bad enough, the paper, presumably written as an attempt in trying to save infants' lives, ironically was ultimately used as a defense in cases where parents were suspected in multiple deaths of their own children in cases of Münchausen syndrome by proxy. The 1972 Pediatrics' paper was cited by 404 papers in the interim and is still listed on Pubmed without comment.[37]

Consequences for those who expose misconduct

The potentially severe consequences for individuals who are found to have engaged in misconduct also reflect on the institutions that host or employ them and also on the participants in any peer review process that has allowed the publication of questionable research. This means that a range of actors in any case may have a motivation to suppress any evidence or suggestion of misconduct. Persons who expose such cases, commonly called whistleblowers, can find themselves open to retaliation by a number of different means.[27] These negative consequences for exposers of misconduct have driven the development of whistle blowers charters - designed to protect those who raise concerns. A whistleblower is almost always alone in his fight - his career becomes completely dependent on the decision about alleged misconduct. If the accusations prove false, his career is completely destroyed, but even in case of positive decision the career of the whistleblower can be under question: his reputation of "troublemaker" will prevent many employers from hiring him. There is no international body where a whistleblower could give his concerns. If a university fails to investigate suspected fraud or provides a fake investigation to save their reputation the whistleblower has no right of appeal. High profile journals like Nature and Science usually forward all allegations to the university where the authors are employed, or may do nothing at all.

Exposure of falsified data

With the advancement of the internet, there are now several tools available to aid in the detection of plagiarism and multiple publication within biomedical literature. One tool developed in 2006 by researchers in Dr. Harold Garner's laboratory at the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas is Déjà Vu, an open-access database containing several thousand instances of duplicate publication. All of the entries in the database were discovered through the use of text data mining algorithm eTBLAST, also created in Dr. Garner's laboratory. The creation of Déjà Vu and the subsequent classification of several hundred articles contained therein have ignited much discussion in the scientific community concerning issues such as ethical behavior, journal standards, and intellectual copyright. Studies on this database have been published in journals such as Nature and Science, among others.[38][39]

Other tools which may be used to detect falsified data include error analysis. Measurements generally have a small amount of error, and repeated measurements of the same item will generally result in slight differences in readings. These differences can be analyzed, and follow certain known mathematical and statistical properties. Should a set of data appear to be too faithful to the hypothesis, i.e., the amount of error that would normally be in such measurements does not appear, a conclusion can be drawn that the data may have been forged. Error analysis alone is typically not sufficient to prove that data have been falsified, but it may provide the supporting evidence necessary to confirm suspicions of misconduct.

Data sharing

Kirby Lee and Lisa Bero suggest, "Although reviewing raw data can be difficult, time-consuming and expensive, having such a policy would hold authors more accountable for the accuracy of their data and potentially reduce scientific fraud or misconduct."[40]

Individual cases

Research conducted during employment by an institution or a corporation

In China

  • H. Zhong, T. Liu, and their co-workers at Jinggangshan University have retracted numerous papers published in Acta Crystallographica following systematic checking which revealed that the organic structures claimed in these papers were impossible or implausible. The supporting data appeared to have been taken from valid cases which had then been altered by substituting different atoms into the structures.[41][42]

Denmark

Germany

Great Britain

Japan

Netherlands

Norway

  • A researcher employed by a Norwegian hospital (Stavanger universitetssjukehus) analyzed samples of spinal fluid from patients, after the researcher had added a substance to the sample.[58]
  • Jon Sudbø fabricated data for a study that reported "nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs reduced the risk of oral cancer".[59]

South Africa

  • Werner Bezwoda, who admitted to scientific misconduct in trials on high-dose chemotherapy on breast cancer.[60]

South Korea

Spain

  • Juan Carlos Mejuto and Gonzalo Astray[62] (chemical physics). Two papers in Journal of Chemical and Engineering Data withdrawn by the editor[63] because of plagiarism.

Switzerland

United States

[80] [81]

Non-institutional and non-corporate research

  • Cyril Burt was an English educational psychologist who made contributions to educational psychology and statistics.Burt is known for his studies on the heritability of IQ. Shortly after he died, his studies of inheritance and intelligence came into disrepute after evidence emerged indicating he had falsified research data.[82]

See also

Categories

Notes

  1. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite pmid}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by PMID 10406378 , please use {{cite journal}} with |pmid=10406378 instead.
  2. ^ "Coping with fraud" (PDF). The COPE Report 1999: 11–18. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2007-09-28. Retrieved 2006-09-02. It is 10 years, to the month, since Stephen Lock ... Reproduced with kind permission of the Editor, The Lancet.[dead link]
  3. ^ http://arstechnica.com/science/2008/08/what-are-the-consequences-for-scientific-misconduct/
  4. ^ "Scientific Misconduct: Do the Punishments Fit the Crime?" Redman and Berz Sciencemag Vol 321 http://astro.berkeley.edu/~kalas/ethics/documents/redan08.pdf
  5. ^ Research Triangle Institute "Consequences of Whistleblowing for the Whistleblower in Misconduct in Science Cases " 1995 https://wiki.umn.edu/pub/IBS8099F10/WhistleBlowing/RTI_-_Consequences_of_Whistleblowing_report.pdf
  6. ^ Goodstein, David (January–February 2002). "Scientific misconduct". Academe. AAUP.
  7. ^ Phenomenon of Collective Nobel-Prize Paranoia vs Phenomenon of Nobel-Prize Hunger
  8. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0005738, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0005738 instead.
  9. ^ New Research Misconduct Policies, NSF
  10. ^ 45 CFR Part 689 [1]
  11. ^ Shapiro, M.F. (1992). "Data audit by a regulatory agency: Its effect and implication for others" (PDF). Accountability in Research. 2 (3): 219–229. doi:10.1080/08989629208573818. PMID 11653981.
  12. ^ Emmeche, slide 5
  13. ^ Eugene Garfield (January 21, 2002). "Demand Citation Vigilance". The Scientist 16(2):6. Retrieved 2009-07-30. {{cite web}}: Italic or bold markup not allowed in: |publisher= (help)
  14. ^ Emmeche, slide 3, who refers to the phenonemon as Dulbecco's law.
  15. ^ Template:Cite PMID
  16. ^ a b Template:Cite PMID
  17. ^ "Publication Ethics Policies for Medical Journals — The World Association of Medical Editors". Wame.org. Retrieved 2009-07-30.
  18. ^ "Publication Ethics Policies for Medical Journals — The World Association of Medical Editors". Wame.org. Retrieved 2009-07-30.
  19. ^ http://www.icmje.org/#author
  20. ^ "Publication Ethics Policies for Medical Journals — The World Association of Medical Editors". Wame.org. Retrieved 2009-07-30.
  21. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite doi}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by doi:10.1136/jme.2004.010553, please use {{cite journal}} (if it was published in a bona fide academic journal, otherwise {{cite report}} with |doi=10.1136/jme.2004.010553 instead.
  22. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite pmid}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by PMID 15238595, please use {{cite journal}} with |pmid=15238595 instead.
  23. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite pmid}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by PMID 9112845, please use {{cite journal}} with |pmid=9112845 instead.
  24. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite pmid}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by PMID 18092023, please use {{cite journal}} with |pmid=18092023 instead.
  25. ^ Wired Magazine, March 2004
  26. ^ [2][dead link]
  27. ^ a b c "Lessons from the Pearce affair: handling scientific fraud". BMJ (310): 1547. June 17, 1995. Retrieved 2009-09-08. (requires free registration)
  28. ^ "Independent Committee of Inquiry into the publication of articles in the British Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology (1994-1995)". Retrieved 2011-08-26. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  29. ^ Wilmshurst P. Institutional corruption in medicine (2002). British Medical Journal; 325:1232-5
  30. ^ a b Template:Cite PMID
  31. ^ http://retractionwatch.wordpress.com/2012/06/13/shikeagi-kato-who-resigned-post-in-march-retracts-nature-paper/
  32. ^ Attention: This template ({{cite pmid}}) is deprecated. To cite the publication identified by PMID 10735823, please use {{cite journal}} with |pmid= 10735823 instead.
  33. ^ Nicholas Wade (2006-01-24). "It May Look Authentic; Here's How to Tell It Isn't". New York Times. Retrieved 2010-04-01.
  34. ^ "Nih Guide: Final Findings Of Scientific Misconduct". Grants.nih.gov. Retrieved 2009-07-30.
  35. ^ a b Steinschneider A (1972). "Prolonged apnea and the sudden infant death syndrome: clinical and laboratory observations". Pediatrics. 50 (4): 646–54. PMID 4342142. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  36. ^ a b Talan, Jamie; Firstman, Richard (1997). The death of innocents. New York: Bantam Books. ISBN 0-553-10013-0.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  37. ^ Prolonged apnea and the sudden infant death syndro...[Pediatrics. 1972] - PubMed Result
  38. ^ Errami M, Garner HR (2008-01-23). "A tale of two citations". Nature. doi:10.1038/451397a. {{cite web}}: Italic or bold markup not allowed in: |publisher= (help); Missing or empty |url= (help)
  39. ^ Long TC, Errami M, George AC, Sun Z, Garner HR (2009-03-06). "SCIENTIFIC INTEGRITY: Responding to Possible Plagiarism". Science. doi:10.1126/science.1167408. {{cite web}}: Italic or bold markup not allowed in: |publisher= (help); Missing or empty |url= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  40. ^ Ethics: Increasing accountability Nature (2006) | doi:10.1038/nature05007
  41. ^ William T. A. Harrison,a Jim Simpsonb and Matthias Weilc (January 2010). "Editorial". Acta Crystallographica Section E. 66: e1–e2. doi:10.1107/S1600536809051757. ISSN 1600-5368.
  42. ^ Doreen Walton (8 January 2010). Lancet urges China to tackle scientific fraud. BBC.
  43. ^ http://universitypost.dk/taxonomy/term/809
  44. ^ Fraud investigation rocks Danish university, from Nature.
  45. ^ Milena Penkowa in Danish-language Wikipedia
  46. ^ "Scandal Rocks Scientific Community". Deutsche Welle. 30 September 2002.
  47. ^ Jo Revill (2006-01-15). "Doctor in drug research row quits NHS post". London: Observer. Retrieved 2009-09-08.
  48. ^ Phil Baty (2006-01-06). "Drugs trial row scientist resigns". Times Higher Education. Retrieved 2009-09-08. did not indicate that initial inquiries uncovered wrongdoing
  49. ^ Actonel Case Media Reports - Scientific Misconduct Wiki
  50. ^ Cook, N. "Breathe easy?" RoSPA Occupational Safety Health J., November 2011, pp. 9-13.
  51. ^ Learmount, D. "Cabin-air research seems to reverse no-risk conclusion" Flight International, 15-21 November 2011, p. 16.
  52. ^ Ramsden, J.J. (2011), "The scientific adequacy of the present state of knowledge concerning neurotoxins in aircraft cabin air", J. Biol. Phys. Chem., 11: 152-164.
  53. ^ University of Tsukuba (2007-11-27). "Investigation Report on the Suspected Scientific Misconduct" (PDF). Retrieved 2010-05-05.Hiroshi Mizubayashi. "University of Tsukuba defends professor's dismissal". Physics Today. doi:10.1063/1.3086082.
  54. ^ "Access : Doubts over biochemist's data expose holes in Japanese fraud laws". Nature. Retrieved 2009-07-30. {{cite web}}: Italic or bold markup not allowed in: |publisher= (help)
  55. ^ D. Normile. "Scientific Misconduct. Japan's Universities Take Action". Science Magazine. doi:10.1126/science.315.5808.26. {{cite web}}: Italic or bold markup not allowed in: |publisher= (help)
  56. ^ S. Shafer (2012). "Statement of Concern" (PDF).
  57. ^ Gretchen Vogel (31 October 2011). "Dutch 'Lord of the Data' Forged Dozens of Studies". Science. Retrieved 2 November 2011.
  58. ^ http://www.vg.no/nyheter/innenriks/artikkel.php?artid=10040433 "Etter press innrømmet forskeren tidlig i vår at han hadde tilsatt et stoff i ryggmargsvæsken for å oppnå resultater som ville vekke oppsikt og sikre hans posisjon som forsker."
  59. ^ Altman, Lawrence K. (2006-05-02). "For Science's Gatekeepers, a Credibility Gap". The New York Times.
  60. ^ Weiss RB, Rifkin RM, Stewart FM, Theriault RL, Williams LA, Herman AA, Beveridge RA. (2000-03-18). "High-dose chemotherapy for high-risk primary breast cancer: an on-site review of the Bezwoda study". The Lancet. 355 (9208): 999–1003. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(00)90024-2. PMID 10768448.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  61. ^ a b Altman, Larry (May 2, 2006). "For Science's Gatekeepers, a Credibility Gap". New York Times. Retrieved 2008-03-26. Recent disclosures of fraudulent or flawed studies in medical and scientific journals have called into question as never before the merits of their peer-review system. The system is based on journals inviting independent experts to critique submitted manuscripts. The stated aim is to weed out sloppy and bad research, ensuring the integrity of the what it has published. {{cite news}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |coauthors= (help)
  62. ^ Rivera, Alicia (May 20, 2011). "Ciencia china 'duplicada' en Galicia". El País.Ingendaay, Paul (June 15, 2011). "War die Guttenberg-Affäre denn zu gar nichts gut?". Frankfurter Allgemeine Zeitung.
  63. ^ "Addition/Correction: Prediction of Refractive Index of Polymers Using Artificial Neural Networks". Journal of Chemical & Engineering Data. 56 (3): 688. 2010-02-09. doi:10.1021/je200071p."Addition/Correction: Prediction of Prop-2-enoate Polymer and Styrene Polymer Glass Transition Using Artificial Neural Networks". Journal of Chemical & Engineering Data. 56 (3): 689. 2010-02-09. doi:10.1021/je200072e.
  64. ^ http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3808019
  65. ^ UConn Investigation Finds That Health Researcher Fabricated Data (The Chronicle of Higher Education, January 11, 2012)
  66. ^ Harvard Dean Confirms Misconduct in Hauser Investigation, Science, August 20, 2010
  67. ^ http://ori.dhhs.gov/misconduct/cases/Gonzalez.shtml
  68. ^ http://ori.dhhs.gov/documents/annual_reports/ori_annual_report_2008.pdf
  69. ^ "Grad student falsified data", The Scientist, 5 August 2008.
  70. ^ [http://blog.al.com/birmingham-news-stories/2009/12/ex-uab_researchers_work_may_be.html Ex-UAB researcher's work may be fake (The Birmingham News December 08, 2009
  71. ^ At Lawrence Berkeley, Physicists Say a Colleague Took Them for a Ride George Johnson, The New York Times, 15 October 2002
  72. ^ Reich, Eugenie. "Biologist spared jail for grant fraud". Nature. doi:10.1038/474552a. Retrieved 1 May 2012.
  73. ^ "Near-Closure of a University Chemist Plagiarism Case". ScienceWeek. 1998-03-20. Retrieved 2009-09-08. [The NSF and Paquette] have apparently agreed to a legally binding settlement, in which Paquette excludes himself from receiving any federal funding for the next 2 years, while NSF agrees not to issue a finding of scientific misconduct. ... The university's chemistry department, however, considers the plagiarism charge insignificant, saying that Paquette's actions "could be considered sloppy, but do not constitute plagiarism by most definitions." {{cite web}}: Italic or bold markup not allowed in: |publisher= (help)
  74. ^ "Office of Research Integrity Newsletter" (PDF). Retrieved 2009-07-30.
  75. ^ "Top Pain Scientist Fabricated Data in Studies, Hospital Says", Wall Street Journal, 11 March 2009.
  76. ^ "Nih Guide: Findings Of Scientific Misconduct". United States Department of Health and Human Services. 2001-12-13. Retrieved 2009-09-08. NOTICE: NOT-OD-02-020
  77. ^ Bridget Murray (February 2002). "Research fraud needn't happen at all". Monitor on Psychology. 33 (2). American Psychological Association. Retrieved 2009-09-08.
  78. ^ http://www.albany.edu/~scifraud/data/sci_fraud_4555.html
  79. ^ http://retractionwatch.wordpress.com/2012/02/22/tenth-potti-retraction-appears-in-clinical-cancer-research/
  80. ^ http://grants.nih.gov/grants/guide/notice-files/not95-262.html
  81. ^ http://www.thecrimson.com/article/1995/12/12/nih-cites-two-researchers-for-misconduct/
  82. ^ http://www.indiana.edu/~intell/burtaffair.shtml
  83. ^ Hick JF (1973). "Letter to editor: Sudden Infant Death Syndrome and Child Abuse". Pediatrics. 52 (1): 147. PMID 4724436. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  84. ^ Steinschneider A (1994). "Prolonged apnea and the sudden infant death syndrome: clinical and laboratory observations". Pediatrics. 93 (6): 944. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  85. ^ Lucey JF (1994). "Woman Confesses in Deaths of Children". Pediatrics. 93 (6): 944. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  86. ^ General Medical Council, 24 May 2010. "Dr. Andrew Jeremy Wakefield: Determination on Serious Professional Misconduct (SPM) and Sanction". Accessed on 10 August 2011 at: http://www.gmc-uk.org/Wakefield_SPM_and_SANCTION.pdf_32595267.pdf

References

  • Broad, William & Wade, Nicholas (1982). Betrayers of the Truth. Oxford University Press.
  • Kilbourne, Brock K. & Kilbourne, Maria T. (1983). The Dark Side of Science, Proc. of the 63rd Annual Meeting of the Pacific Division, AAAS, April 30, 1983.
  • Claus Emmeche. "An old and a recent example of scientific fraud" (PowerPoint). Retrieved 2007-05-18.
  • Mounir Errami, Justin M. Hicks, Wayne Fisher, David Trusty, Jonathan D. Wren, Tara C. Long, and Harold R. Garner. (2007). "Déjà vu – A Study of Duplicate Citations in Medline". Bioinformatics, December 2007.
  • Kochan Carol Ann, Budd John M (1992). "The persistence of fraud in the literature: The Darsee Case". Journal of the American Society for Information Science. 43 (7): 488–493.
  • Gerald P. Koocher and Patricia Keith-Spiegel, "Peers Nip Misconduct in the Bud", Nature, vol. 466, 22 July 2010. pp. 438–440.
  • Patricia Keith-Spiegel, Joan Sieber, and Gerald P. Koocher (November, 2010). Responding to Research Wrongdoing: A User Friendly Guide.
  • Bratislav Stankovic (2004) "Pulp Fiction: Reflections on Scientific Misconduct", 2004 Wisconsin Law Review 975.

External links